PARTCH'S ERRORS
by
B. McLaren
Harry Partch stands as one of the most influential innovators in 20th century music. However, his book Genesis of A Music is riddled with psychoacoustic errors.
The gross pervasive errors of fact in his writings so flagrantly contradict so many documented facts about the human ear that they offer a minefield for prospective non-12 composers.
Without exception, his statements on psychoacoustics embody the exact opposite of the truth. In short, if Partch said it about the human ear, you can count on the reverse being factually correct.
Why was Partch so reliably and so completely wrong when it came to matters of musical perception?
For one thing, he listened exclusively to harmonic-series timbres when tuning sustained tones on his instruments. For another, Partch does not appear to have been familiar with any of the psychoacoustic literature published after 1945--the Stone Age of psychoacoustics.
Partch's information was woefully out of date. He penned "Genesis of a Music" between 1928 and 1947, an era when ignorance of the ear/brain system reigned. In the early 1930s no one had heard of the critical band, no one realized that the ear hears stretched octaves and stretched fifths and stretched thirds as "just," and "pure," while hearing purportedly "natural" small-integer-ratio fifths and thirds and octaves as "too narrow" and "impure" and "out of tune." Prior to 1945, no one imagined that the rules of harmony changed radically as soon as sustained timbres became inharmonic.
Thus Partch completely missed out on the enormous post-World War II explosion of knowledge in modern psychoacoustics.
The modern era of ear/brain research dates from the period 1954 onward--in particular, from Max Mathews' seminal paper in Bell Labs Technical Journal in 1959. Because computers can generate sounds with any desired spectrum and amplitude, and because the computer peerlessly analyzes acoustic sounds in precise detail, our knowledge of the ear/brain system increased explosively during the late 1960s and throughout the seventies.
Partch blithely ignored all of the advances in musical psychoacoustics from 1928 through 1974. And so his pervasively false claims about the behavior of the human ear serve as a warning to prospective just intonation composers: before you embarrass yourself by making provably untrue pronouncements about the human ear/brain system, it's best to do some research first and get your facts straight.
"The number of cycles--per second--determines the pitch of the tone." Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd Ed., Da Capo Press: New York, 1974, pg. 76.
"For sine tones in particular, the pitch does not depend exclusively on the frequency; the intensity is also relevant." - Johan Sundberg, "The Science of Musical Sounds," Academic Press, Inc.: San Diego, 1991, pg. 45.
"The pitch of pure tones depends not only on frequency, but also on other parameters such as sound pressure level." H. Zwicker & . Fastl, "Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models," 1993, pg. 105.
"The pitch of harmonic complex tones depends on level. Figure 4.9 shows the pitch shift of a complex tone with a 200-Hz fundamental frequency as a function of its level. An increasing negative pitch shift shows up with increasing level of the complex tone." H. Zwicker & E. Fastl, "Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models," 1993, pg. 111.
"After hearing a `major third' on the organ or piano or some other instrument with tempered intonation, this interval becomes fixed in the mind as a pretty poor consonance, at least by some comparisons." Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., pg. 6.
"As will be seen and demonstrated in the recording, there is...reason to doubt the musical merit of a superparticular ratio as low as 4:5." --Fritz Kuttner & J. Murray Barbour, program notes, Musurgia Records' series on the History of Musical Theory. In "Theory and Practice of Just Intonation."
"A small difference can be seen for the fifth, the dyad version being close to the barbershop result of 705 cents. One can also see that these musicians preferred the Pythagorean major third to the just." - Johan Sundberg, "The Science of Musical Sounds," pg. 101.
"The average extent of each of the five intervals approximated its theoretical magnitude in Pythagorean intonation. The unquestionably crucial cases were those of the minor second, the minor third, and the major third, where Pythagorean intonation differed significiantly from natural and equally tempered intonation. Since the performers on the average apprixmated the Pythagorean and larger natural scale values of the major second, and did not significantly differentiate between the two theoretical varieties of this interval in the natural scale, it may be justifiable to assume that the criterion in actual performance here also was Pythagorean intonation." Greene, Paul C. "Violin intontion," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol, 9, 1937, pp. 43-44
"This 2 to 1 relationship is a constant one...the fact is that nature does not offer one tone and its doubling (200 to 400) as a given quality of relationship, and the same quality of relationship in two tones which are not a ratio of doubling (200 to 600, for example)" Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd Ed., Da Capo Press: New York, 1974, pg. 77.
"...a soft sine tone at 300 Hz may sound as a pure octave of a loud sine tone at 168 Hz. The mathematically pure octave, however, has the frequency of 150 Hz. The tone that sounds as a pure octave is 12% too high. This means that mathematically it is a minor seventh [300/188 = 10.04 semitones]! This is a good argument for avoiding confusion of perceptual and physical entities." - Johan Sundberg, The Science of Musical Sounds," Academic Press, Inc.: San Diego, 1991, pg. 46.
"If a frequency of 8 kHz is chosen for f1, subjects produce for the sensation of `half pitch' not a frequency of 4 kHz, but a frequency of about 1300 Hz." E. Zwicker and H. Fastl, Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models," 1993, pg. 103.
"Dowland has reported that measurements of Western and non-Western fixed pitch instruments support Ward's conclusion that the perceptual octave is some 15 cents larger than the physical or mathematical octave. Western musical practice supports these conclusions (play sharp in higher octave). Balinese gamelan tunings take advantage of this apparently widespread characteristic of pitch perception to create a multi-octave beating complex in their fixed pitch instruments." [Erickson, R., "Timbre and the Tuning of the Balinese Gamelan," Soundings, Vol. 15, pg. 100, 1984]
"The ear informs us that tones which are in small-number proportion, say in the relation of 2 to 1, are strong, clear, powerful, consonant." Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., Da Capo Press: New York, 1974, pg. 86.
"It is quite remarkable that musicians seem to prefer too wide or `stretched' intervals... In the case of octaves, the craving for stretching has been noticed for both dyads and melodic intervals. the amount of stretching preferred depends on the mid frequency of the interval, among other things. The average for synthetic, vibrato-free octave tones has been found to be about 15 cents. Thus subjects found a just octave too flat but an octave of 1215 cents [= 2.017403967581/1 = approx. 115/57 or 117/58] just." -- Johan Sundberg, "The Science of Musical Sounds," Academic Press, Inc.: San Diego, 1991, pg. 103.
"The experimental results very convincingly show that, on the average, singers and string players perform the upper notes of the major third and the major sixth with sharp intonation (Ward 1970)...The same experiments revealed that also fifths and fourths and even the almighty octave were played or sung sharp, on the average! (A reciprocal effect exists. Pure octaves are consistently judged by musicians to sound flat!) Rather than revealing a preference for a given scale (the Pythagorean), these experiments point to the existence of a previously unexpected universal tendency to play or sing sharp all musical intervals." - Juan Roederer, "Introduction to The Physics and Psychophysics of Music," 1973, pg. 155.
"These results confirm the view that, for simple tones, tonal consonance is related to interval width. The experiment does not support the hypothesis that the human ear is provided with some sort of frequency-ratio detector." Plomp, R., W. A. Wagenaar and A. M. Mimpen. "Musical Interval Recognition with Simultaneous Tones," Acustica, Vol. 29, 1973, pp. 100-109
"Consequently, these statements can be conclusively made; the ear consciously or unconsciously classifies intervals according to their comparative consonance or comparative dissonance; this faculty in turn stems directly form the comparative smallness or comparative largeness of the numbers of the vibrational ratio..." Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., pg. 87.
"In general, the observation that simple tones can be differentiated according to ratio simplicity is largely an artifact, due to the correlation between ratio simplicity and interval width." Levelt, W. J. M., J. P. van de Geer and R. Plomp, "Triadic Comparisons of Musical Intervals," the British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology," Vol. 19, part 2, 1966, pp. 163-179
"In fact, the interval that listeners accept as the best octave is not the interval with a precisely two-to-one frequency ratio but one with a slightly larger and thus numerically much more complex ratio (Ward, 1954; also see Burns, 1974; Dowling, 1973b, in press; Elfner, 1964; Sundberg & Lindqvist, 1973; Ward & Martin, 1961 Chapter 10, this volume). And the frequencies of the tones actually produced by musicians or preferred by listeners as representative of other musical intervals, as well, often fail to conform to the predicted most simple ratios (Seashore, 1938; Ward & Martin, 1961; Van Esbroeck & Montfort, cited in Risset, 1978)." [Cazden, Norman, Musical Consonance and Dissonance: A Cultural Criterion," Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, Vol. 4, No. 1, 1945, pg. 9]
"Therefore it must be concluded that even just or pythagorean intonation cannot be considered as ideal. Rather, optimum intonation of a diatonic scale probably depends on the structure of the actual sound in the same manner as has been previously discussed with respect to tempered scales." -- E. Terhard and S. Zick, "Evaluation of the Tempered Tone Scale In Normal, Stretched, and Contracted Intonation," Acustica, Vol. 32, 1975, pg. 273.
"The degree of consonance depends on the quality or spectrum of the component tones, i.e., the relative intensity of dissonant vs. consonant upper harmonics." - Juan Roederer, "Introduction to the Physics and Psychophysics of Music," 1973, pg. 143.
"It became clear that the fifth (2:3) is not always a consonant interval. A chord of two tones that consists of only odd harmonics, for example, shows much worse consonance at the fifth (2:3) than at the major sixth (3:5) or some other frequency ratios. This was proved true by psychological experiments carried out in another institutes (Sensory Inspection Committee in the Japan Union of Scientists and Engineers) with a different method of scaling. Thus, the fact warns against making a mistake in applying the conventional theory of harmony to synthetic musical tones that can take on a variety of harmonic structures." Akio Kameoka and Mamoru Kuriyagawa, "Consonance Theory Part II: Consonance of Complex Tones and Its Calculation Method," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 45, No. 6, 1969, pg. 1460.
"Long experience in tuning reeds on the Chromelodeon convinces me that it is preferable to ignore partials as a source of musical materials. The ear is not impressed by partials as such. The faculty--the prime faculty--of the ear is the perception of small-number intervals, 2/1, 3/2, 4/3, etc., etc., and the ear cares not a whit whether these intervals are in or out of the overtone series." Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., pg. 87.
"In 1987 IPO issued a wonderful disc by Houtsma, Rossing and Wagenaars...illustrating the effects of a moderate stretching...of scale frequencies and/or partial spacings. Part of a Bach chorale is played with synthesized tones. When neither scale nor partial frequencies are stretched, we hear the intended harmonic effect. When the scale is unstretched but the partial frequencies are stretched, the music sounds awful. Clearly, intervals in the ratio of small whole numbers are in themselves insufficient to give Western harmonic effects." -- John R. Pierce, "The Science of Musical Sound," 2nd Ed., 1992, pp. 91-92.
"Clearly the timbre of an instrument strongly affects what tuning and scale sound best on that instrument." Wendy Carlos, "Tuning: At the Crossroads," Computer Music Journal, 1987.
"For two thousand years music theorists searched for a `natural' explanation of musical pitch systems and syntax. Their point of departure was, as a rule, some sort of acoustical data--the lengths of vibrating strings, the overtones series, or some other natural property of sound. Using such data, an attempt was made to show that this or that system as natural--and hence, by extension, necessary and valid. But the development of new tonal systems in the West, the study of the history of Western music, and research in comparative musicology made it clear that musical styles are not natural forms of communication, but are learned and conventional." -- Leonard B. Meyer, "Music, the Arts and Ideas," 1967, p. 73.
"Most instruments in our music culture produce harmonic spectra, as mentioned. However, in the contemporary computer-aided electroacoustic music studios, is not a necessary constraint any longer. One would then ask if this does not open up quite new possibilities also with respect to harmony. If one decides to use one particular kind of inharmonic spectra for all tones, it should be possible to tailor a new scale and a new harmony to this inharmonicity." - Johan Sundberg, "The Science of Musical Sounds," Academic Press, Inc.: San Diego, 1991, pg. 100.
"By using a digital computer, musical tones with an arbitrary distribution of partials can be generated. Experience shows that, in accord with Plomp's and Levelt's experiments with pairs of sinusoidal tones, when no two successive partials are too close together such tones are consonant rather than dissonant, even though the partials are not harmonics of the fundamental. For such tones, the conditions for consonance of two tones will not in general be the traditional ratios of the frequencies of the fundamentals... [The 8-TET scale] is , of course, only one example. If many possible scales made up of tones whose upper partials are not harmonics of the fundamental and having unconventional intervals, which nonetheless can exhibit consonance and dissonance comparable to that obtained with conventional musical instruments (which have harmonic partials) and the diatonic scale. It appears that, by providing music with tones that have accurately specific but nonharmonic partial structures, the digital computer can release music from the constraint of 12 tones without throwing consonance overboard." John R. Pierce, "Attaining Consonance in Arbitrary Scales," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1966, p. 249.
"That the inception of this scale was a significant insight is abundantly supported by the fact that the first three of its ratios [2/1, 3/2, 4/3] are the most important scale degrees in nearly every musical system worthy of the name that the world has ever known." Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., pg. 87.
"As to whether the interval 3:2 is common to all of the world's musical systems, as has occasionally been claimed, Fritz Kuttner asserts that the "fifth" in Chinese music is 20 to 30 cents flat. It is apparently nearly as flat in Siamese music..." M. Joel Mandelbaum, "Multiple Division of the Octave and the Tonal Resources of 19-Tone Temperament," 1960, p. 16.
"Two theoretical systems evolved in China, one derived from the Cyclic Pentatonic and the other from the division of string lengths. They are found combined in the highest form of Ch'in music. (..) Methods of arriving at these fifths included the use of twelve tubes... The fifths produced by these tubes were small compared to Western fifths. Various musicologiests place them between 670 and 680 cents as compared to the Just fifth of 702 cents." [Lentz, Donald A., The Gamelan Music of Java and Bali, 1965, pg. 27]
"There are...a number of musical cultures that apparently employ approximately equally tempered 5- and 7-interval scales (i.e., 240 and 171 cent step-sizes, respectively) in which the fourths and fifths are significantly mistuned form their natural values. Seven-interval scales are usually associated with Southeast Asian cultures (Malm, 1967). For example, Morton (1974) reports measurements (with a Stroboconn) of the tuning of a Thai xylophone that `varied only + or - 5 cents' from an equally tempered 7-interval tuning. (In ethnomusicological studies measurement variability, if reported at all, is generally reported without definition.) Haddon reported (1952) another example of a xylophone tuned in 171-cent steps from the Chopi tribe in Uganda. The 240-cent step-size, 5-interval scales are typically associated with the `gamelan' (tuned gongs and xylophone-type instruments) orchestras of Java and Bali (e., Kunst, 1949). However, measurements of gamelan tuning by Hood (1966) and McPhee (1966) show extremely large variations, so much so that McPhee states: `Deviations in what is considered the same scale are so large that one might with reason state that there are as many scales as there are gamelans.' Another example of a 5-interval, 24--cent step tuning (measured by a stroboconn, 'variations' of 15 cents) was reported by Wachsmann (1950) for a Ugandan harp. Other examples of equally tempered scales are often reported for pre-instrumental cultures... For example, Boiles (1969) reports measurements (with a Stroboconn, `+ or - 5 cents accuracy') of a South American Indian scale with equal intervals of 175 cents, which results in a progressive octave stretch. Ellis (1963), in extensive measurements in Australian aboriginal pre-instrumental cultures, reports pitch distributions that apparently follow arithmetic scales (i.e., equal separation in Hz).
"Thus there seems to be a propensity for scales that do not utilize perfect consonances and that are in many cases highly variable, in cultures that either are pre-instrumental or whose main instruments are of the xylophone type. Instruments of this type produce tones who partials are largely inharmonic (see Rossing, 1976) and whose pitches are often ambiguous (see de Boer, 1976)." [Burns, E. M. and Ward, W. D., "Intervals, Scales and Tuning," in The Psychology of Music, 1982, ed. Diana Deutsch, pg. 258]
"The acoustically 'perfect' consonances are the rule in some musics, but are not inevitable foundations, for nothing close to the ratio 3:2 is found in certain Javanese and Siamese scales. Intervals which bear no resemblance to any in our diatonic system form melodies which to their users seem 'instinctive' and self-evidently natural. Harmony in seconds, which we would consider flagrantly dissonant, seems to be practiced in the South Sea Islands and elsewhere. In the Icelandic 'Tvisoengvar' the third appears to be treated as a dissonance. It could hardly be maintained that the natural laws presumed to determine harmony vary thus geographically in their application." [Cazden, Norman, Musical Consonance and Dissonance: A Cultural Criterion," Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, Vol. 4, No. 1, 1945, pg. 9]
"3. Preference ratings that listeners give for pairs of tones that do stand in the various simple frequency ratios tend to depart systematically from the orderings of those ratios predicted on the basis of what is usually taken to be their numerical simplicities-with, for example, the major third (5:4) often preferred to the numerically simpler perfect fourth (4:3) (e.g., Butler & Daston, 1968; Krumhansl & Shepard, 1979; see also Davies, 1978, p. 158; Fuda, 1975; Van de Geer, Levelt, & Plomp, 1962).
"The mean ratings of consonance indicate that there are large differences between consonance as defined by earlier judgments of musicians steeped in beat lore and by laymen in the present experiment. The octave, that most consonant of intervals to the music theoretician, got only an average consonance rating of 4.6, being exceeded by 9 others. The intervals judged most consonant, at 5.7, were the ratios 5:7 and 5:8 -- a 583-cent tritone and an 814-cent minor sixth, respectively. The cellar (2.0) was occupied by the four smallest intervals (8:9, 9:10, 10:11, and 11:12 -- 204, 182, 165, and 150 cents, respectively). ...It seems clear that these results do not agree with the rules of consonance laid down by the small-integer proponents. It is therefore unnecessary, in explaining consonance, to appeal to neuromythological structures such as a series of brain circuits tuned to each frequency, so that 'in the case of the fifth, 3:2, every third wave of the higher note and every second wave of the lower could use the same circuit, and so on' (Boomsliter and Creel, 1961, pg. 20)."[Ward, W.D., "Musical Perception," in Foundations of Modern Auditory Theory, ed. Jerry V. Tobias, Vol. I. Academic Press: New York, 1970, pg. 435]
"In any given range of pitch the comparative consonance of an interval is determined by the relative frequency of the wave period in the sounding of the interval." Harry Partch, "Genesis of Music," 2nd. Ed., Da Capo Press: New York, 1974, pg. 151.
"Systematic measurements show that people tend to find that an interval traditionally classified as `consonant' sounds progressively dissonant the farther down in the bass it is played...In the very low bass, even the octave sounds dissonant!" Johan Sundberg, "The Science of Musical Sounds," Academic Press, Inc.: San Diego, 1991, pg. 73.
"Even the order in which two instruments define a musical interval is relevant. For instance, if a clarinet and a violin sound a major third, with the clarinet playing the lower note, the first dissonant pair of harmonics will be the 7th harmonic of the clarinet with the 6th harmonic of the violin (because only odd harmonics of the clarinet are present). This interval sounds smooth. If, on the other hand, the clarinet is playing the upper tone, the 3rd harmonic of the latter will collide with the 4th harmonic of the violin tone, and the interval will sound harsh.'" - Juan Roederer, "Introduction to The Physics and Psychophysics of Music," 1973, pg. 143.
"[The ear] can determine almost immediately, exactly or approximately, the relationship in vibrations per second, or cycles, of two tones sounded simultaneously; it can say instantly whether the two tones are in the correct ratio (in tune) or not in the correct ratio (out of tune)." Harry Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., pg. 86.
"Time is a fundamental limitation on the ability to perceive pitch. When a tone sounds, a certain time must pass before the listener develops a sensation of pitch. The length of this time depends upon the frequency of the tone. In order to establish a pitch, a listener must receive a number of cycles of a tone. Thus it takes a longer time to perceive the pitch of a tone at a lower frequency because that tone has a longer period. For example, a tone must last at least 40 msec at 100 Hz whereas a tone at 1000 Hz must last only 13 msec." - Charles Dodge & Thomas A. Jerse, "Computer Music: Synthesis, Performance and Composition," 1985, pg. 46.
"The handling and consideration of tones is, by virtue of their vibrations, an exact mathematical process. If a tone makes 200 cycles, the 2/1 ("octave") above it makes 400 cycles, a doubling of 200, or 200 more cycles." [Genesis of a Music, 2nd. Ed., 1974, pg. 77]
"In fact, the interval that listeners accept as the best octave is not the interval with a precisely two-to-one frequency ratio but one with a slightly larger and thus numerically much more complex ratio (Ward, 1954; also see Burns, 1974; Dowling, 1973b, in press; Elfner, 1964; Sundberg and Lindqvist, 1973; Ward & Martin, 1961, Chapter 10, this volume). And the frequencies of the tones actually produced by musicians or preferred by listeners as representative of other musical intervals, as well, often fail to conform to the predicted most simple ratios (Seashore, 1938; Ward & Martin, 1961; Van Esbroeck & Montfort--cited in Risset, 1978). [R. N. Shepard, "Structural Representations of Musical Pitch," pg. 347, in "The Psychology of Music," Ed. Diana Deutsch, 1982]
"It is instructive to examine a few intervals between pure tones that occur in different octaves. If C4 (262 Hz) and G4 (392 Hz) are sounded together, the difference frequency is 130 Hz, which is 40 percent greater than the critical bandwidth (approximately 90 Hz in this octave). Thus they sound consonant. However, an octave lower on the scale, a perfect fifth is not quite so consonant, since the frequency difference between C3 (131 Hz) and G3 (196 Hz) is 65 Hz, which is less than the critical bandwidth. Another octave or so lower, the frequency difference approaches 1/4 of the critical bandwidth, the criterion for maximum roughness or dissonance. So the degree of dissonance of the interval between two pure tones is strongly dependent on their location on the musical scale." [Rossing, "The Science of Musical Sound," 2nd. Ed., 1990, pg. 157]
"There is just one element in all the study of music which does not change: the ratio. And the reason the ratio does not change is simply and wholly because physiologically the ear does not change except over a period of thousands or millions of years." [Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., 1974, pg. 97]
"Experiments with inharmonic partials (Slaymaker, 1970; Pierce, 1966) have shown that consonance is indeed dependent on the coincidence of partials and not necessarily on the simple frequency ratio between the fundamental frequencies (which is usually the cause of the coincidence)." [R.A. Rasch & R. Plomp, "The Perception of Musical Tones," pg. 21, in "the Psychology of Music," Ed. Diana Deutsch, 1982]
"The scale of musical intervals begin with absolute consonance (1 to 1) and gradually progresses into an infinitude of dissonance, the consonance of the intervals decreasing as the odd number of their ratios increase." [Partch, "Genesis of a Music," 2nd. Ed., 1974, pg. 87]
"As Risset (1978) has remarked, ratios that are very close to simple ratios (e.g., 29,998:20,000) are highly complex and yet indiscriminable from the corresponding simple ratios (in this case 3:2)... Preference ratings that listeners give for pairs of tones that do stand in the various simple frequency ratios tend to depart systematically from the orderings of those ratios predicted on the basis of what is usually taken to be their numerical simplicities--with, for example, the major third (5:4) often preferred to the numerically simpler perfect fourth (4:34) (e.g., Butler & Daston, 1968; Krumhansl & Shepard, 1979; see also Davies, 1978, p. 158; Fyda, 1975; Van de Geer, Levelt & Plomp, 1962). [R. N. Shepard, "Structural Representations of Musical Pitch," pg. 347, in "The Psychology 9f Music," Ed. Diana Deutsch, 1982]
Again let me point out that Partch was a genius. This does not change the fact that almost everything he wrote about psychoacoustics was dead wrong.
Still--even though Partch's arguments in favor of just intonation are based on a set of numerological canards and psychoacoustic falsehoods--this does NOT make Partch's tuning system useless or ugly-sounding or pointless. Just intonation remains a beautiful and musically interesting "angle of hearing." Much uniquely interesting music can be composed using just intonation; this alone is reason to use JI in composition, and excellent reason to listen to music composed with small integer ratios.
Just intonation offers many advantages which other tuning systems do not: for example, each transposition of a JI melody to another pitch produces a new and unique distribution of intervals, and this makes iterated sequences of melodic notes, which would sound repetitious in an equal temperament, extremely interesting in just intonation.
Using small integer ratios to generate a scale also allows the composer to produce startling and dramatic effects when modulating to another key.
Integer ratios also allow a composer to climb the harmonic series directly, exploring radically new and non-western musical intervals: the 11/8, the 13/8, the 17/11, the 19/16, the 31/16, and so on ad infinitum.
Most of all, just intonation allows the composer to use exotic and hitherto-undreamt-of mathematical techniques to generate radically new tunings of breathtaking beauty. For example, Erv Wilson's combination-product-set scale generation techniques, John Chalmers' tritriadic methods, juggler numbers, Farey series, Pascal's Triangle, hailstone numbers, and many other numerical series and methods.
Just intonation also suffers many unique disadvantages. Modulation to other keys demands a scale with many more notes than the usual 12 of western music. There are also no rule-books or instructional texts for dealing with ratios made up of higher members of the harmonic series: is the 19/16, for example, a consonance or a dissonance? How ought the 7:9:11 chord to be used in a musical context? And so on.
While it remains apodictically true that non-equal non-just tunings better fit the human tendency to hear stretched intervals as "pure" than other types of tunings, this does not render other types of tunings musically invalid. As John R. Pierce, Wendy Carlos, myself, Bill Sethares, Johan Sundberg and many others have pointed, consonance and dissonance are determined by musical context, the tuning used, and the precise spectral and phase characteristics of the timbres in question.
Witch-hunts for "good" and "bad" tunings are futile. All three general classes of tunings [just, equal tempered, non-just non-equal-tempered] sound dramatically different, and provide equally limitless musical resources for the xenharmonic composer.
While Harry Partch's statements about psychoacoustics uniformly conflict with the facts, this does not mean that his music is worthless. Forget Partch's book...celebrate his music. The music is what matters.
APPENDIX
Predictably, this article will spark the usual firestorm of protests from just intonation enthusiasts unwilling to accept the proven facts of the human ear/brain system. Such protests are symptoms of the appalling ignorance of today's purportedly "educated" musicians. Contemporary musicians are not to blame: their ignorance results from the disgracefully inadequate state of musical "education" throughout the western world. In so-called institutions of "higher learning," music is still taught as an intellectually toxic witch's brew of numerology, superstition and acoustic fairytales. The startling and fascinating results of the psychoacoustic research carried out over the last 40 years are uniformly ignored by textbooks on so-called "contemporary" music, with the inevitable consequence that graduates from the world's most presitigious musical institutions remain shockingly ignorant of how their own ears work
Those who doubt these statements are advised to study the following extremely abbreviated list of references:
Allen, D. "Octave discriminability of musical and non-musical subjects," Psychonomic Science, Vol. 7, 1967, pp. 421-422
Appleton, J., "Machine Songs III: Music in the Service of Science--Science in the Service of Music," Computer Music Journal, 13(3), Fall 1992, pp. 17-21
Attneave, F. and Olson, R. "Pitch as a medium: A new approach to psychophysical scalaing," Am. J. of Psychology, Vol. 84, 1971, pp. 146-166
Balzano, G. "On the bases of similarity of musical intervals: A chronometric analysis," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 61, 1977, S51(A)
Balzano, G. J. "The pitch set as a level of description for studying musical pitch perception," in M. CLynes (Ed.,), Music, mind, and brain: the neurophysiology of music. New York: Plenum, 1982, pp. 321-351
Bastiaans, M. J., "Gabor's expansion of a signal into Gaussian elementary signals," Proceedings of the IEEE, Vol. 68, 1980, pp. 538-539
Becker, Judith. "Is western art music superior?" Musical Quarterly, Vol. 72, No. 2, 1986, pp. 341-359
Bekesy, G. von, "Three Experiments Concerned with Pitch Perception," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 35, 1963, pp. 1722-1724.
Bekesy, G. von, "Hearing Theories and Complex Sounds," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 35, No. 4, pg. 589, April 1963
Bekesy, G. von, "Ueber die nichtlinearen Verzerungen des Ohres," Ann. Phys., Vol. 20, pp. 809-827, 1934
Bekesy, G. von, "Sensations on the Skin Similar to Directional Hearing, Beats, and Harmonics of the Ear," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 29, pp. 489-501, 1957
Bekesy, G. von, "Ueber akustische Rauhigkeit, " Z. techn. Phys., Vol. 16, pp. 276-282, 1935
Bekesy, G. von, "Concerning the Fundamental Component of Periodic Pulse Patterns and MOudlated Vibrations Observed on the Cohlear Model with Nerve Supply," J. Acooust. Soc. Am., Vol. 33, 1961, pp. 888-896
Bernstein, Leslie R. and David M. Green. "Detection of simple and complex changes of spectral shape," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 82, No. 5, 1987, pp. 1587-1265
Bilsen, F. A., "Pitch Perceptibility for Multi-Component Signals," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 51, 1971, 113(A)
Bismarck, G. von, "Sharpness as an attribute of the timbre of steady sounds," Acustica, Vol. 30, 1974, pp. 159-172
Boomsliter, P., and Creel. W., "The long pattern hypothesis in harmonya and hearing," Journal of Music Theory, Vol. 5, 1961, pp. 2-31
Boomsliter, P., and Creel. W., "Toward a theory of melody." Paper presented to Symposium on Musical Perception, Convention of the AAS, December 28, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, 1971
Boomsliter, P., and Creel. W., "Hearing with ears instead of instruments," Journal of the Audio Engineering Society, Vol. 18, 1970, pp. 407-412
Bregman, A. S., and Campbell, J. "Primary auditory stream segregation and perpcetion of order in rapid sequences of tones," Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 89, 1971, pp. 244-249
Brown, Helen. "The interplay of set content and temporal context in a functional theory of tonality perception," Music Perception, Vol. 5, No. 3, 1988, pp. 219-250
Bruner, Cheryl L. "The perception of contemporary pitch structures," Music Perception, Vol. 2, No. 1, 1984, pp. 25-39
Burns, E.M. and Ward, W.D., "Intervals, Scales, and Tuning" in The Psychology of Music, ed. Diana Deutsch, 1982, pp. 260 ff.
Burns, E. M. and Ward, W. D., "Categorical Perception -- Phenomneon or Epiphenomenon: Evidence from Experiments In the Perception of Melodic Musical Intervals," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 63(2), pp. 456-468, 1978.
Burns, E. M., and Ward, W. D., "Categorical Perception of Musical Intervals," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 55, pg. 456, 1974.
Buser, P. and Imbert, M., Audition, M.I.T. Press, 1995
Burns, E. M., and Ward, W. D., "Perception of monotic and dichotic musical intervals," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 59, 1976, 552(A)
Burns, E. M. "The perception of musical intervals (frequency ratios). Unpublished doctoral tehesis, University of Minnesota, 1977
Burns, Edward M. and Neal F. Viewmeister, "Nonspectral pitch," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 60, No. 4, 1976, pp. 863-869
Burns, E. M., "In search of the Shruti," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 56, Supplement, 1974, S26(A)
Butler, David and W. Dixon Ward. "Effacing the memory of musical pitch," Music Perception, Vol. 5, No. 3, 1988, pp. 251-260
Callow, G. and Shepherd, E. "Intonation in the performance of North Indian classical music." Paper presented at the 17th Annual meeting of the Society for Ethnomusicology, November 30-December 3, Toronto, Canada, 1972
Caron, N. and Safvate D. Les traditions musicales, Iran. Correa, France: Buchet/Chastel, 1966
Cazden, N., "Sensory Theories of Musical Consonance," J. Aesth. Art Criticism, 20, pp. 301-319
Cazden, N., "Musical Consonance and Dissonance: A Cultural Criterion," J. Aesthet., Vol. 4, pp. 3-11, 1945
Cazden, N., "Musical Intervals and Simple Number Ratios," J. Res. Music Educ., 7, pp. 197-220.
Christman, R. J. and Williams, W. E., "Influence of the Time Interval on Experimentally Induced Shifts of Pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 35, pp. 1030-1033, 1963
Cohen, A., "Further Investigation of the Effects of Intensity Upon the Pitch of Pure Tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 33, pp. 1363-1376, 1961
Cohen, Elizabeth, "Fusion and Consonance Relations for TOnes with Inharmonic Partials," J. Acoust. SOc. Amer. Vol. 65, S123, 1970 (abstract)
Cohen, Elizabeth, Ph. D. Thesis, Stanford University, 1980.
Corso, J.F., "Unison Tuning of Musical Instruments," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 26, pp. 746-750, 1954
Cross, D., and Lane, H. "Attention to Single Stimulus Properties in the Identification of Complex Tones," in Experimental Analysis of the Control of Speech Produciton and Perception, University of Michigan ORA Rep. No. 05613-1-P, 1963
Cross, I., R. West and P. Howell. "Pitch relations and the formation of scalar structure," Music perception," Vol. 2, No. 3, 1985, pp. 329-344
Crowder, Robert G. and Ian Neath. "The influence of pitch on time perception in short melodies," Music perception, Vol. 12, No. 4, 1994, pp. 379-386
Cutting, J. and Roener, B., "Categories and Boundaries in SPeech and Music," Percept. Psychophysics, VOl. 16, 1974, pp. 564-570
Danner, G. "The use of acoustic measures of dissonance to characterize pitch-class sets," Music Perception, Vol. 3, No. 1, 1985, pp. 103-122
Darwin, C. J., R. W. Hukin and Batul Y. Al-Khatib. "Grouping in pitch perception: evidence for sequnetial constraints," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 98, No. 2, part 1, 1995, pp. 880-885
Davis, H., S. R. Silverman and D. R. McAuliffe, "Some Observations on Pitch and Frequency," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 23, pp. 40-42, 1951
de Boer, E. "On the Residue in Hearing," Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of Amsterdam, 1956.
de Boer, E., "Pitch of Inharmonic Signals," Nature, Vol. 178, 1956, pp. 535-536
Deutsch, Diana and Richard C. Boulanger. "Octave equivalence and the immediate recall of pitch sequences," Music Perception, Vol. 2, No. 1, 1984, pp. 40-51
Deutsch, Diana. "Mapping of interactions in the pitch memory store," Science, Vol. 175, 1972, pp. 1020-1022
Deutsch, Diana, "The Processing of Pitch Combinations," in D. Deutsch, (Ed.), The Psychology of Music, New York: Academic Press, 1982.
Deutsch, Diana. "A musical paradox," Music perception, Vol. 3, No. 3, 1986, pp. 275-280
Deutsch, Diana. "Interference in memory between tones adjacent in the musical scale," Jounral of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 100, No. 2, 1973, pp. 228-231
Deutsch, Diana. "Music perception," The Musical Quarterly, Vol. 66, No. 2, 1980, pp. 165-179
Deutsch, D., "Dichotic listening to musical sequences. Relationship to hemispheric specialization of function," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 74, 1983, pp. 579-80.
Deva, B. C., "The Vibrato in Indian Music," Acustica, 9, pp. 175-180, 1959.
Dobbins, P. A. and Cuddy, L. L., "Octave disrimintion: an experimental confirmation of the `stretched' nonsubjective octave," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 72, 1982, ppp. 411-415
Doughty, J. M. and Gardner, W. R., "Pitch Characteristics of Short Tones - II. Pitch As A Function of Tonal Duration," J. Exp. Psychol., 38, 1948, pp. 478-494
Dowling, W. J. "The 1215-Cent Octave: Convergence of Western and Non-Western Data on Pitch Scaling," Abstract QQ5, 84th meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Friday, December 1, 1972, p. 101 of program.
Dowling, W. J. "Scale and countour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies," Psychological review, Vol. 85, No. 4, 1978, pp. 341-354
Dowling, W. Jay. "Tonal strength and melody recognition after long and short delays," Perception and Psychophysics, Vol. 50, No. 4, 1991, pp. 305-313
Dowling, W. J. and Diane S. Fujitani. "Contour, Interval and pitch recognition in memory for melodies," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 49, No. 2, Part 2, 1971, pp. 524-531
Duifuis, H., "Audibility of High Harmonics in A Periodic Pulse," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 48, No 4., Part II, 1970, pp. 888-892
Egan, J. P. and Meyer D. R., "Changes in Pitch of Tones Of Low Frequency As A Function of the Pattern of Excitation Produced By A Band of Noise," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 22, 1964, pp. 827-833
Elfner, L., "Systematic Shifts In the Judgment of Octaves of High Frequencies," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 36, 1964, pp. 270-276
Ellis, A. J., "On the Musical Scales Of Various Nations," Journal of the Royal Society of the Arts, Vol. 3, 1885, pg. 536
Ellis, C. "Pre-instrumental scales," Ethnomusicology, Vol. 9, 1965, pp. 126-144
Espinoza-Vargas, Blas, and Donald G. Jamieson. "Integration of spectral and temporal cues separated in time and frequency," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 76, No. 3, 1984, pp. 732-738
Everett, J. D., "On Resultant Tones," Phil. Mag. 5th Ser., Vol. 41, pp. 199-207, 1896
Fetis, F. J., Traite Complet de la Theorie et de la practique de l'Harmonie. Paris: 1875
Flanagan, J. L., and Guttman N., "On the Pitch of Periodic Pulses," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 32, 1960, 1308-1319
Flanagan, J. L., and Guttman, N., "Pitch of Periodic Pulses without Fundamental Component," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 32, 1960, 1319-1328
Flanagan, J. L., "Models for Approximating Basilar Membrane Displacement-Part II," Bell System Tech. J., 41, 939-1009, 1962
Fletcher, H. and Bassett, I. G., "Some experiments with the bass drum," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 64, 1978, pp. 1570-1576
Fletcher, H., Blackham, E. D., and Christensen, D. A., "Quality of organ tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 35, 1963, pp. 314-325
Fletcher, H., Blackham, E. D. and Stratton, R., "Quality of piano tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 34, 1962, pp. 749-761
Fletcher, H., and Sanders, L. C., "Quality of violin vibrato tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 41, 1967, pg. 1334-1344
Fletcher, H., "The Physical Criterion for Determining the Pitch of a Musical Tone," Phys. Rev., Vol. 23, pp. 427-437
Fletcher, H., "A Space-Time Pattern Theory of Hearing," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 1, pp. 311-343, 1930
Frances, R. La perception de la musique. Paris: Vrin, 1958
Fransson, F., Sundberg, J. and Tjernlund, P., "Statistical Computer Measurements of the Tone-Scale in Played Music," Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress and Status Report 2-3 (Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm, Sweden), 1962
Freeman, M. D., "Analysis of musical isntrument tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 41, 1967, pp. 793-806
Gabor, Denis., "Acoustical Quanta and the Theory of Hearing," Nature, Vol. 159, pg. 303, 1947.
Garner, W. R., "Statistical Aspects of Loudness Judgments," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 24, pp. 153-157, 1957.
Geary, J.M., "Consonance and Dissonance of Pairs of Inharmonic Sounds," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 67(5), May 1980, pp. 1785-1789
Gerson, A. and Golstein, J. L. "Evidence for a general template in central optimal processing for pitch of complex tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 63, 1978, pp. 498-510
Goldstein, J. L., "An Investigation of Monarual Phase Perception." Doctoral Dissertation, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York, 1965
Goldstein,, J. L. "Aural COmbination Tones," in Frequency Analysis and Periodicity Detection in Hearing, R. Plomp and G. F. SMoorenburg, Eds. Sythoff, Leiden, The Nethlands, 1970.
Goldstein, J. L, Baer, T., and Kiang, N.Y.S. "A Theoretical Treatment of Latency, Group-Delay and TUning Characteristics for Auditory Nerve Responses to Clicks and Tones," in Physiology of the Auditory System, M. B. Sachs, Ed., National Educational Consultants, Baltimore, Maryland, 1971.
Goldstein, J. L. and Kiang, N.y.S, "Neural Correlates of the Aural Combination Tone 2f1-f2," Proceedings of the IEEE, Vol. 54, 1968, pp. 981-992
Greene, P. C. "Violin intonation," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 9, 1937, pp. 43-44
Green, David M. "Temporal Acuity as a function of frequency," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 54, No. 2, 1973, pp. 373-379
Green, David M., "Discrimination of Transient signals having identical energy spectra," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 48, No. 4, Part 2, 1970, pp. 894-905
Greenwood, D., "Critical beandwidth and consonance in relation to cochlear frequency-position coordinates," Hearing Research, Vol. 54, No. 2, 1991, pp. 164-208
Grey, J. M., An Exploration of Musical Timbre, Ph. D., dissertation, Dept. of Psychology, Stanford University: Dept. of Music Rep. STAN-M-2, 1975
Grey, J. M., "Multidimensional perceptual scaling of musical timbres," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 61, 1977, pp. 1270-1277
Grey, J. M., "Timbre discirmination in musical patterns," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 64, 1978, pp. 467-472
Gulick, W. L., George A. Geschneider and Robert D. Frisina, Hearing: Physiological Acoustics, Neural Coding and Psychoacoustics, Oxford University Press, 1989
Haddon, E. "Possible origin of the Chopi Timbala xylophone," African Music Society Newsletter, Vol. 1, 1952, pp. 61-67
Hagerman, B. and Sundberg, J., "Fundamental Frequency adjustment in barbershop quartet singing," Journal of Research in Singing, Vol. 4, 1980, pp. 3-17
Hantz, Edwin. "Studies in musical cognition: comments from a music theorist," Music Perception, Vol. 2, No. 2, 1984, pp. 245-264
Hartmann, William Morris, Stephen McAdams and Bennett K. Smith. "Hearing a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic complex tone," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 88, No. 4, 1990, pp/ 1712-1724
Helmholtz, H. L. F., von, "Ueber Combinationstoene," Ann. Phys. Chem., Vol. 99, pp. 497-540, 1856
Helmholtz, H L. F. von, Die Lehre von den Tonempfindungen als physiologische Grundlage fuer die Theorie der Musik. F. Vieweg & Sohn, Braunschweig (Germany), 1st Ed., 1863.
Helmholtz, H. L. F. von, On the Sensations of Tone, Dover (2nd) edition, 1954, with foreword, afterword and footnotes by Alexander J. Ellis.
Hiller, L. and Ruiz z, R., "Synthesizing Sounds by Solving the Wave Equation for Vibrating Objects," Journal of the Audio Engineering Society, Vol. 19, pp. 463-470, 1971
Hirsh, Ira J. "Auditory perception of temporal order," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, VBol. 31, No. 6, 1959, pp. 759-767
Hood, M. "Slendro and Pelog redefined," in Selected Papers in Ethnomusicology, Institute of Ethnomusicology, UCLA, VOl. 1, 1966, pp. 36-48
Houtsma, A. J. M., "Perception of harmonic intervals made by simultaneous complex tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol., 73, 1983, S77
Houtsma, A. J. M., "Musical pitch of two-tone complexes and predicitions by modern pitch tehories," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 66, 1979, pp. 87-99
Houtsma, A. J. M., "Discrimination of frequency ratios," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 44, 1968, 383(A)
Houtsma, A.J.M, and J.L. Goldstein, "The Central Origin of the Pitch of Complex Tones: Evidence from Musical Interval Recognition," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 51, No. 2, 1972, pg. 520
Houtsma, A. J. M. and J. Smurzynski. "Pitch identification and discrimination for complex tones with many harmonics," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 87, No. 1, 1990, pg. 304-310
Houtsma, A. J. M, "What Determines Musical Pitch?" J. Mus. Theory, 17(1), 1973, pp. 138-155
Houtsma, A. J. M., "Auditory Discrimination of Frequency Ratios," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 24, pp. 153-157, 1952.
Huron, D., "Tonal consonance versus tonal fusion in polyphonic sonorities," Music Perception, Vol. 9, no. 2, 1991, pp. 135-154
Huron, D., and Sellmer, P., "Critical bands and the spelling of vertical sonortiies," Music Perception, Vol. 10, No. 2, 1992, pp. 129-149
Husmann, H. Vom Wesen der Konzonanz. Heidelberg: Verlag Mueller-Thiergarten, 1953
Hutchinson, W. and Knopoff, L. "The acoustic component of Western consoannce," Interface, Vol. 7, No. 1, 1978, pp. 1-29
Hutchinson, W. and Knopoff, L. "The significance of the acoustic component of consonance in Western triads," Journal of Musicological Research, Vol. 3, 1979, pp. 5-22
Jacobs, J. E., and Wittman, P. "Psychoacoustics, The Determining Factor In Stereo Disc Recording," J. Audio Enginneering. Soc., 12, 1964, pp. 115-123
Jansson, F. and Sundberg, J., "Long-term average spectra applied to analysis of music," Acustica, Vol. 34, 1975/1976, pp. 15-19, 269-374
Jhairazhboy, N., and Stone, A. "Intonation in Present Day North Indian Classical Music," Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies, Vol. 26, University of London, 1963, pp. 118-132
Jordan, Daniel S. "Influence of the diatonic tonal hierarchy at microtonal intervals," Perception and psychophysics, Vol. 41, No. 6, 1987, pp. 482-488
Kallman, H. J., "Octave equivalence as measured by similarity ratings," Perception and Psychophysics, Vol. 32, 1982, pp. 37-49
Kameoka, A., and Kuriyagawa, M., "Consonance Theory Part I: Consonance of Dyads," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 45, No. 6, 1969, pp. 1451-1459
Kameoka, A., and Kuriyagawa, M., "Consonance Theory Part II: Consonance of Complex Tones and Its Calculation Method," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 45, No. 6, 1969, pp. 1460-1469
Keeler, J. A., "Piecewise periodic analysis of almost-periodic sounds and musical transients," IEEE Transactions on Audio and Electroacoustics, AU-ZO, 1972, pp. 338-344
Killam, R., Lorton, Pr., and Schubert, E. "Interval Recognition: Identification of Harmonic and Melodic Intervals," J. Mus. Theory, Vol. 20, 1976, pp. 212-234
Kock, Winston E. "On the principle of uncertainty in sound," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 7, 1936, pp. 56-58
Koening, "Effect of Time On Pitch Discrimination Thresholds Under Several Psychophysical Procedures; Comparison With Intensity Discrimination Thresholds," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 29, 1957, pp. 606-612
Koester, T., and W. N. Schoenfeld. "The effect of context upon judgments of pitch differences," Journal of Experimental SPcyhology, Vol. 36, 1946, pp. 417 ff.
Krumhansl, C. Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1990
Krumhansl, Carol L. and Paul Iverson. "Perceptual interations between musical pitch and timbre," Jounral of Experimental SPcyhology: Human perceprtion aqnd performance, Vol. 18, No. 3, 1992, pp. 739-751
Kubovy, Michael and Ray Jordan. "Tone-segregation by phase: on the phase sensitivity of the single ear," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 66, No. 1, 1979, pp. 100-106
Lauter, J. L., "Stimulus characteristics and relative ear advantages: a new look at old data," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 74, 1983, pp. 1-17
Lawrence, M. and P. A. Yantis, "Thresholds of Overload in Normal and Pathological Ears," A. M. A. Arch. Otol., Vol. 63, pp. 67-77, 1956
Lawrence, M. and P. A. Yantis, "Onset and Growth of Aural harmonics in the Overlaoded Ear," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 28, pp. 852-858, 1956
Lawrence, M. and P. A. Yantis, "In Support of an `Inadequate' Method for Detecting `Fictitious' Aural Harmonics," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 29, pp. 750-751, 1957
Levelt, W. J. M., J. P. can de Geer, and R. Plomp, "Triadic Comparisons of Musical Intervals," Brit. J. Mathem. Statist. Psychol., 1966
Levitt, H., "Transformed Up-Down Methods In Psychoacoustics," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 49, pp. 467-477, 1971
Lichte, W. H., "Attributes of Complex Tones," J. Exp. Psychol., 28, 1941, pp. 455-480
Lichte, W. H. and Gray, R. F., "Influence of Overtone Structure On the Pitch Of Complex Tones," J. Exp. Psychol., 49, 1941, pp. 431-436
Licklider, J. C. R., "Periodicity pitch and `place' pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 26, 1954, 127(A)
Licklider, J. C. R., "The duplex theory of pitch perception," Experientia 7, 1951, pp. 128-134
Licklider, J. C. R., "Periodicity Pitch and Related Auditory Process Models," Int. Audiol., Vol. 1, 1962, pp. 11-35
Licklider, J. C. R., "Auditory Frequency Analysis," in Information Theory, ed. C. Cherry, Buttersworths Scientific Publications, London, pp. 253-268, 1956
Lloyd, Llewyelyn S. "Musical theory in retrospect," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 13, 1941, pp. 56-62
Locke, S. and Ekllar, "Categorical Perception in a Non-Linguistic Mode," Cortex, Vol. 9, 1973, pp. 355-369
Longuet-Higgins, H. C. "The perception of music," Proceedings of the Royal Society London, Vol. 205 B, 1979, pp. 307-322
McAdams, S., Spectral Fusion, Spectral Parsing and the Formation of Auditory Images. PhD. diss., Stanford University, 1984.
McAdams, S., "The Auditory Image: A Metaphor for Musical and Psychological Research on Auditory Organization," in W. R. Crozier and A. J. Chapman, eds., Cognitive Processes in the Perception of Art. Amsterdam, North Holland, 1984
McPhee, C. Music in Bali. New Haven, Connecticut: Yale University Press, 1966
Majernik, V. and J. Kaluzny. "On the auditory uncertainty relations," Acustica, Vol. 13, 1979, pp. 132-146
Malm, W. P. Music cultures of the Pacific, the Near East, and Asia. Englewood CLiffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1967
Malmberg, C. F., "The perception of consonance and dissonance," Psychological Monographs, Vol. 25, 1918, pp. 93-133
Martin, D. W., "Musical scales since Pythagoras," Sound, Vol. 1, 1962, pg. 22
Martin, D. W., "Decay rates of piano tones," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 19, 1947, pg. 335
Martin, D. W and Ward, W. D., "Subjective evaluation of musical scale temperament for pianos," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 33, 1961, pg. 582
Mason, J. A., "Comparison Of Solo and Ensemble Performances With Reference To Pythagorean, Just and Equi-Tempered Intonations," J. Res. Music. Educ., 8, 1960, pp. 31-38
Mathes, R. C., and Miller, R. L., "Phase effects in monaural perception," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 19, 1947, pp. 780-797
Mathews, M. V., and Kohut, J., "Electronic simulation of violin resonances," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 53, 1973, pp. 1620-1626
Mathews, M. V., Miller J. E., Pierce, J. R., and Tenney, J., Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 38, 1965, pg 912 (abstract only)
Mathews, M.V., Current Directions In Computer Music, MIT Press, 1989
Mathews, M., "The Digital Computer As a Musical Instrument," Science, Vol., 142, November, 1963, pg. 554
Mayr, Albert. "New and rediscovered Zeitgebers in recent music," Contemporary music review," Vol. 7, 1993, pp. 79-89
Meyer, Leonard B., Music, The Arts and Ideas, 1967
Meyer, M. "Zur Theorie der Differenztoene und der Gehorseempfindungen ueberhaupt," Beitraege Akustische Musikwissenschaft, Vol. 2, 1898, pp. 25-63
Miller, G. A., "The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information," Psychological Review, Vol. 63, 1956, pp. 81-96
Moe, C. R., "An Experimental Study of Subjective Tones Produced Within the Human Ear," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 14, 1942, pp. 159-166
Moore, Brian C. J., Brian R. Glasberg and Robert W. Peters. "Relative dominance of individual partials in determining the pitch of complex tones," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 77, No. 5, 1985, pp. 1853-1860
Moore, Biran C. J., Brian R. Glasberg and Robert W. Peters. "Thresholds for hearing mistuned partials as separate tones in harmonic complexes," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 80, No. 2, 1986, pp. 479-483
Moorer, J. A., "The use of the phase vocoder in computer music applications," Journal of the Audio Engineering Society, Vol. 26, 1978, pp. 42-43
Moran, H, and C. C. Pratt, "Variability of Judgments on Musical Interval," J. Exp. Psych., 9, 1926
Moreno, E., Expanded Tunings in Contemporary Music, 1992, The Edwin Mellen Press
Morgan, C. T., Garner, W. R., and Galambos, R., "Pitch and Intensity, " J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 23, 1951, pp. 658-663
Morton, D., "Vocal tones in Thai music," Selected Reports in Ethnomusicology (Volume 2), Los Angeles, California Institute for Ethnomusicology, UCLA, 1974, pp. 88-99
Nickerson, James F. "Intonation of solo and ensemble performance of the same melody," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 21, No. 6, 1949, pp. 593-595
Nickerson, James F., "A Comparison of Performances Of the Same Melody Played In Solo and In Ensemble With Reference to Equi-Tempered, Just and Pythagorean Intonations." Unpublished doctoral Dissertation, University of Minnesota.
Nordmark, Jan O. "Mechanisms of Frequency Discirmination," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 44, No. 6, 1968, pp. 1533-1540
Ohgushi, K., "The origin of tonality and a possible explanation of the octave enlargement phenomenon," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 73, 1983, pp. 1694-1700
Ohm, G. S., "Noch ein Paar Worte ueber die Definition des Tones," Ann. Phys. Chem., Vol. 62, pp. 1-18, 1844
Ohm, G. S., "Bermerkungen ueber Combinationstoene und Stoesse," Ann. Phys. Chem., Vol. 47, pp. 463-466, 1839.
Ohm, G.S., "Ueber die Definition des Tones, nebst daran geknuepfer Theorie der Sirene und aehnlicher tonbildener Vorrichtungen," Ann. Phys. Chem., Vol. 59, pp. 513-565
Parncutt, R., Harmony: A psychoacoustical approach. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1989
Parncutt, Richard. "Pitch properties of chords of octave-spaced tones," Contemporary Music Review, Vol. 9, Parts 1 & 2, 1993, pp. 35-50
Patterson, Roy D. and Frederic L .Wightman. "Residue pitch as a fucntion of component spacing," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 59, No. 6, 1976, pp. 1450-1459
Patterson, James H. and David M. Green. "Discrimination of transient signals having identical energy spectra," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 48, No. 4, part 2, 1970, pp. 895-905
Patterson, Roy D. "Noise masking of a change in residue pitch," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 45, No. 6, 1969, pp. 1520-1524
Penner, M. J. and Edward Cudahy. "Critical masking interval: a temporal analog of the critical band," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 54, No. 6, 1973, pp. 1530-1534
Perlman, M., "The American Gamelan and the Garden of Eden: Intonation In A Cross-Cultural Collision," Musical Quarterly, 1995, pg. 243
Pickles, J. O., An Introduction to the Physiology of Hearing, 2nd ed., Academic Press, New York, 1988
Pierce, J.R., "Attaining Consonance In Arbitrary Scales," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 39, pg. 249, 1966
Pierce, J. R., and M. V. Mathews, "Control of Consonance and Dissonance with Nonharmonic Overtones," in Music From Computers, ed. von Foerster, H., and James W. Beauchamp, John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1969, pp. 129-132
Pierce, J. R., The Science of Musical Sound, 2nd edition, 1992
Pierce, J. R., R. Lipes and C. Cheetham. "Uncertainty concerning the direct use of time information in hearing: Place cues in white-spectra stimuli," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 61, No. 6, 1977, pp. 1609-1621
Pierce, John R. "Periodicity and pitch perception," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 90, No. 4, part 1, 1991, pp. 1889-1893
Pierce, John R. "Rate, place and pitch with tonebursts," Music perception," Vol. 7, No. 3, 1990, pp. 205-212
Pierce, J.R. , Mathews, M. and Risset J-C., "Further Experiments on the Use of the Computer in Connection with Music," Grasvener Blaetter, no. 27/28, 1965, pg. 93
Pikler, A. G., "History of Experiments on the Musical Interval Sense," J. Mus. Theory, 10, 1966.
Pikler, A. G., "Logarithmic Frequency Systems," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 39, 1966
Pikler, A. G., and J. D. Harris, "Measurement of the Musical Interval Sense," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 23, 1961
Pikler, Andrew J. "Mels and musical intervals," Journal of Music Theory," Vol. 10, 1966, pp. 289 sqq.
Pisoni, D. B. and Lazarus, J. "Categorical and non-categorical modeso of speech perception along with the voicing continuum," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 55, 1974, pp. 328-333
Plomp, R. and Levelt, W. J. M., "Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 35, 1965, pp. 548-560
Plomp, R. "Beats of mistuned consonances," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 42, 1967, pp. 442-474
Plomp, R. and Steeneken, J. M., "Effect of phase on the timbre of complex tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 1969, Vol. 46, pp. 409-421
Plomp, R., "The Ear As a Frequency Analyzer," J. acoust. Soc. Am., 36, 1964, pp. 1628-1636
Plomp, R., "Detectability Threshold For Combination Tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 37, 1965, pp. 1110-1123
Plomp, R., "Pitch Of Complex Tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 41, 1967, pp. 1526-1533
Plomp, R., "Rate of Decay of Auditory Sensation," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 38, pp. 277-282, 1964
Plomp, R., and M. A. Bouman, "Relation Between Hearing Threshold and Duration for Tone Pulses," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 31, 749-748, 1959
Plomp, R. Wagenaar, W. and Mimpen, A., "Musical Interval Recognition with Simultaneous Tones," Acustica, Vol. 29, 1973, pp. 101-109
Plomp, R., Experiments On Tones Perception. Netherlands: Van Gorcum & Comp. N.V, 1966
Poland, W., "Theories Of Music and Musical Behavior," J. Music Theory, 7 1963, pp. 150-173
Pollack, Irwin. "The information of elementary auditory displays," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 24, No. 6, 1952, pp. 745-749
Pollack, I., "Ohm's Acoustical Law and Short-Term Auditory memory," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 36, 1964, pp. 2340-2345
Pratt, C. C., "Some qualitative aspects of intoned complexes," American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 32, 1921, pp. 490-515
Preusser, David. "The effect of structure and rate on the recognition and description of auditory temporal patterns," Perception and psychophysics, Vol. 11, No. 3, 1972, pp. 233-239
Railsback, O. L., "Scale temperament as applied to piano tuning," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 9, 1938, pg. 274
Rakowski, A., "Timing of isolated musical intervals," Journ. Acout. Soc. Am., Vol. 59, 1974, S50
Rakowski, A. "Context-dependent intonation variants of melodic intervals," in Music, Language, Speech and Brain, ed. Johan Sundberg, Lennart Nord and Roald Carlson, MacMillan Press, 1991.
Rasch, R., "A perception of melodic and harmonic intonation of two-part musical fragments," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 74, 1983, S22
Rasch, R. A., and Plomp, R., "The Perception of Musical Tones," pp. 1-21, in The Psychology of Music, ed. Diana Deutsch, 1982
Risset, J.C. "Computer Music Experiments: 1964..." Computer Music Journal, Vol. 9, No. 1, 1985, pg. 14
Risset, J.C., "The Development of Digital Techniques: A Turning Point for Electronic Music?" Rapports IRCAM No. 9, 1978
Risset, J.C., "Musical Acoustics," Rapports IRCAM No. 8, 1978
Risset, J.C. and Mathews, Max, "Analysis of Musical-Instrument Tones," Physics Today, Vol. 22, No. 2, February 1969, pp. 23-24
Risset, J. C., "Computer Music Experiments 1964...", Computer Music Journal, Vol. 9, No. 1, Spring 1985, pg. 11
Ritsma, R. J., "Frequencies Dominant in the Perception of the Pitch of Complex Sounds," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 42(1), 1967, 191-198
Ritsma, R. J., "Existence Region of the Tonal Residue I," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 34, pp. 1224-1229, 1962
Ritsma, R. J., "Existence Region of the Tonal Residue II," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 35, pp. 1241-1245, 1962
Ritsma, R. J., "Periodicity Detection," in Frequency Analysis and Periodicity Detection in Hearing, R. Plomp and G. F. Smoorenberg, Eds., 1970. A. W. Sijthoff, Leiden, The Netherlands
Roberts, Brian and Peter J. Bailey. "Regularity of spectral pattern and its effects on the perceptual fusion of harmonics," Perception and Psychophysics, Vol. 58, No. 2, 1996, pp. 289-299
Robinson, Ken. "Brightness and spectral position: are changes in spectral envelope and in tone height perceptually equivalent?" Contemporary Music Review, 1993, Vol. 9, Parts 1 & 2, pp. 83-95
Roederer, J., Introduction to the Physics and Psychophysics of Music. Berlin and New York: Springer-Verlag, 1973. 2nd Ed.
Rosenberg, A. E., "Effect of Masking on the Pitch of Periodic Pulses," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 38, 1965, 747-758
Rosner, Burton S. and Eugene Narmour. "Harmonic Closure: music theory and perception," Music Perception, Vol. 9, No. 4, 1992, pp. 383-412
Rossing, T. D., and Neville H. Fletcher, The Physics of Musical Instruments, Springer-Verlag. New York, 1991
Saldanha, F. I. and Corso, J. F., "Timbre cues and the identification of musical instrucments," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., 1964, Vol. 36, pp. 2021-2026
Scharf, B. "Critical bands," in Foundations of modern auditory theory, ed. J. Tobias. Academic Press: new York, Vol. 1, 1970.
Schellenberg, E. Glenn, and Sandra E. Trehub. "Frequency ratios and the dicrimination of pure tones sequences," Perception and psychophysics, Vol. 56, No. 4, 1994, pp. 472-478
Schouten, J. F., "The Residue and the Mechanism of Hearing,"
Proc. Kon. Nederl. Akad. Wetensch., Vol. 43, pp. 991-999, 1940
Schouten, J. R., "The Perception of Pitch," Philips Techn. Rev., Vol. 5, pp. 286-294, 1940
Schouten, J. F., "The Perception of subjective tones," Proceedings of the Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, 1938, vol. 41, pp. 1083-1093.
Schouten, J. F., Ritsma, R. J. And Cardozo, B. Lopes, "Pitch of the Residue," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 34, 1962, 1418-1424
Schroeder, M. R., "Models of hearing," Proceedings of the IEEE, 1975, Vol. 63, pp. 1332-1350
Seashore, C. E. Psychology of music. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1938
Seebeck, A., "Beobachachtungen ueber einge Bedingungen der Enstehung von Toenen," Ann. Phys. Chem., Vol. 53, pp. 4317-436, 1841
Seebeck, A., "Ueber die Sirene," Ann. Phys. Chem., Vol. 60, pp. 449-481, 1843
Seebeck, A., "Ueber die Definition des Tones," Ann. Phys. Chem., Vol. 63, pp. 353-368, 1844
Seebeck., J. T., "Beitraege zur Phyysiologie des Gehoer- and Geschichtissinnes," Poggendorf's Annalen, 68, 1846
Serra, X. and Smith, J.O. "Spectral Modeling Synthesis: a Sound analysis/synthesis System Based on A Deterministic plus Sotchastic Decomposition," Computer Music Journal, Vol. 16, No. 4, pp. 12-24, 1990.
Shackford, R., "Some Aspects of Perception - I," J. Mus. Theory, Vol. 5, 1961, pp. 13-26
Shackford, R., "Some Aspects of Perception - II," Shackford, J. Mus. Theory, Vol. 6, 1962, pp. 13-26
Shackford, R., "Some Aspects of Perception - III," Shackford, J. Mus. Theory, Vol. 6, 1962, pp. 13-26
Shepard, R. N., "Circularity In Judgments Of Relative Pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 36, 1964, pp. 2346-2353
Shepard, R., "Structural Representations of Musical Pitch," in The Psychology of Music, Ed. Diana Deutsch, 1982, pp. 334-373
Shepard, Roger N. and Daniel S. Jordan. "Auditory illusions demonstrating that tones are assimilated to an internalized musical scale," Science, Vol. 226, 14 December 1984, pp. 1333-1334
Siegel, J. A. and Siegel, W., "Cateogrical perception of tonal intervals: Musicians can't tell sharp from flat," Perception and Psychophysics, Vol. 21, 1977, pp. 399-407
Siegel, J. A., and Siegel, W. "Absolute identification of notes and intervals by musicians," Perception and Pscyhophysics," Vol. 21, 1977, pp. 143-144
Simon, Herbert A. and Kenneth Kotovsky. "Human acquisition of concepts for sequential patterns," Psychological review, Vol. 70, no. 6, 1963, pp. 534-546
Simon, Herbert A. "How big is a chunk?" Science, Vol. 183, 8 February 1974, pp. 482-487
Singh, Punita G. "Perceptual organization of complex-tone sequences: a tradeoff between pitch and timbre?" Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 82, No. 3, 1987, pp. 886-899
Slaymaker, F. "Chords from tones having stretched partials, " J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 47, 1978, pp. 1369-1371
Small, Arnold M. Jr. and Max E. McClellan, "Pitch associated with time delay between two pulse trains," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 35, No. 6, 1963, pp. 1246-1255
Small, Arnold M. and Campbell R. A., "Pitch Shifts Of Periodic Stimuli With Changes In Sound Level," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 33, 1961, 1022-1027
Smith, J. O., "Viewpoints on the History of Digital Synthesis," in Proceedings of the 1991 International Computer Music Conference, pp. 1-10, San Francisco: ICMA, 1991.
Smoorenburg, G. F., "Combination Tones and Their Origin," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 52, 1972, pp. 615-632
Smoorenburg, G. F., "Pitch Perception of Two-Frequency Stimuli," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 48, 1970, pp. 924-942
Spector, J., "Classical Ud music in Egypt with special reference to Maqamat," Ethnomusicology, Vol. 14, 1966, pp. 243-257
Stauffer, D., Intonation deficiancies of wind instruments in ensemble. Washington, D. C.: Catholic University of America Press, 1934
Stevens, S. S., in G. Stevens, Ed., Psychophyics: Introduction to its perceptual, neural, and social prospects. New York: Wiley, 1976
Stevens, S. S., "The measurement of loudness," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 27, 1955, pp. 815-829
Stevens, S. , "A scale for the measurement of psychological magnitude: Loudness," Psychological Review, Vol. 43, 1936, pp. 405-416
Stevens, S. S., "On the Validity Of the Loudness Scale," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 31, 1959, pp. 995-1003
Stevens, S. S., "The Basis Of Psychophysical Judgments," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 35, 1963, pp. 611-612
Stevens, S. S., Guirao, M., and Slawson, A. W., "Loudness, a Product of Volume Times Density," J. Exp. Psychol., 69, 1965 pp. 503-510
Stevens, S. S. "Tonal Density," Jounral of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 17, 1934, pp. 585-592
Stevens, S. S., Volkmann, J. and Newman, E. G., "A Scale for the Measurement of the Psychological Magnitude Pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 8, 1937, pp. 185-190
Stumpf, C. Tonpsychologie, Vols. 1 & 2 Verlag S. Hirzel, Leizig (Germany), 1890
Steward, G. W. "Problems suggested by an uncertainty principle in acoustics," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 2, No. 3, 1931, pp. 325-329
Stumpf, C. "Konsonanz und Dissonanz," Beitr. Akust. Musikwiss., Vol. 1, pp. 1-108, 1898
Stumpf, C. "Beobachtungen ueber Kombinationstoene," Z. Psychol., Vol. 55, pp. 1-142, 1910
Stumpf, C. and M. F. Meyer, "Massbestimmungen ueber die Reinheit consonanter Intervalle," Beitraege zur Akustik und Musikwissenschaft, 2, 1898
Summerfield, Quentin, Andrew Sidewll and Tony Nelson. "Auditory enhancement of changes in spectral amplitude," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 81, No. 3, 1987, pp. 700-708
Sundberg, J. E. F., "The acoustics of the singing voice," Scientific American, Vol. 326, 1977, pp. 82-91
Sundberg, J.E. F. & J. Lindqvist, "Musical Octaves and pitch," Journ. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 54, 1973, pp. 922-929
Sundberg, J. E. F., The Science of Musical Sounds, Academic Press, 1992, New York
Sundberg, J. E. F. and Lindqvist, J. "Musical octaves and pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 54, 1973, pg. 822
Sundberg, J. E. F., Studies of Music Performance, Stockholm: Royal Swedish Academy of Music, 1983
Szende, O. Intervallic hearing: Its nature and pedagogy. Budapest: Akademia Kiamo, 1977
Tenney, J. C., "The physical correlates of timbre," Gravesener Blaetter, Vol. 7, Heft 26, pp. 106-109
Terhardt, E., "P{itch shifts of harmonies, an explanation of the octave enlargement phenomenon," Proceedings of the 7th International Congress on Acoustics, Vol. 3, 1971, pp. 621-624
Terhardt, E., "Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Musical Sounds," Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 1978, pp. 483-492.
Terhardt, Ernst. "Pitch shifts of harmonics, an explanation of the octave enlargement pehnomenon," proceedings of the 7th international congress on acoustics, Budapest, Vol. 3, 1971, pp. 621-624
Terhardt, E., "Pitch, Consonance, and Harmony," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 55, 1974, pp. 1061-1960.
Terhardt, E., "Die Tonhoehe Harmonischer Klange und das Oktavintervall," Acustica, 24, 1971, pp. 126-136
Terhardt, E., "Ein psychoakustisch begruendetes Konzept der musikalischen Konsonanz," Acustica, 36, 1976, pp. 121-137
Terhardt, E., "The concept of musical consonance: a link between music and psychoacoustics," Music Perception, Vol. 1, 1984, pp. 276-295
Terhardt, E. "On the perception of periodic sound fluctuation (roughness0," Acustica, Vol. 30, 1973, pg. 201
Thompson, William Forde. "Sensitivity to combinations of musical parameters: pitch with duration, and pitch pattern with durational pattern," Perception and psychophysics, Vol. 56, No. 3, 1994, pp. 363-374
Thurlow, W. R. and Berstein, S., "Simultaneous Two-Tone Pitch Discrimination," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 29, 1957, pp. 515-519
Thurlow, W. R., and Erchul, W. P., "Judged similarity in pitch of octave multiples," Perception and Psychophysics, Vol. 22, 1977, pp. 177-182
Thurlow, W. R. and A M. Small, Jr., "Pitch Perception for Certain Periodic Auditory Stimuli," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 27, pp. 132-137, 1955
Thurlow, W. R., and I. L. Rawlings, "Discirimination of Number of Simultaneously Sounding Tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 31, pp. 1332-1336, 1959
Tobias, J. V., Ed., Foundations of modern auditory Theory. (Volumes 1 & 2) New York: Academic Press, 1970, 1972
Trotter, J. R., "The psychophysics of musical intervals. Definitions, techniques, theory and problems," Australian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 19, 1967, pp. 13-23
Van de Geer, J. P., Levelt, W. and Plomp, R., "The connotation of musical consonance," Acta Pscyhologica, Vol. 20, 1962, pp. 308-319
Van Noorden, L., Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences. Eindhoven, Holland: Instituut voor Perceptic Onderzock, 1975
Vermeulen, R., "Musical Scales," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 20, No. 4, July 1948, pp. 545-549
Vos, J. "Purity ratings of tempered fifths and major thirds," Music Perception, Vol. 3, No. 3, 1986, pp. 221-238
Vos, J. and Ben G. van Vianen. "Thresholds for discrimination between pure and tempered intervals: the relevance of nearly coinciding harmonics," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 77, No. 1, 1985, pp. 176-187
Vos, J. "Spectral effects in the perception of pure and tempered intervals: discrimination and beats," Perception and Psychophysics, Vol. 35, 1984, pp. 173-185
Wachsmann, K., "An equal-stepped tuning in a Ganda harp," Nature (London), Vol. 165, 1950, pg. 40
Wade, B. C. Music in India: the classical traditions. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1979
Walker, James T and Arhur L. Irion. "Two new contingent aftereffects: perceived auditory duration conteingent on pitch and on temporal order," Perceptiona nd psychophysics, Vol. 26, No. 3,1979, pp. 241-244
Ward, W. D., The Subjective Octave and the Pitch of Pure Tones. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.
Ward, W. D., "Subjective Musical Pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 26, 1954, pp. 369-380
Ward, W. D., "Tonal Monaural Diplacusis," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 27, 1955, pp. 365-372
Ward, W. D. and D. W. Martin, "Psychophysical Comparison of Just Tuning and Equal Temperament in Sequences of Individual Tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 33(5), May 1961, pp. 586-588
Ward, W. D., "Musical Perception," pp. 407-447, in Foundations of Modern Auditory Theory, ed. J. V. Tobias. Academic Press: New York, 1970.
Ward, W. D., "Absolute Pitch, Part II," Sound, Vol. 2, 1963, pp. 33-41
Warren, R. M., Auditory Perception: A New Synthesis. Pergamon Press, New York. 1985.
Warren, R. H. "Helmholtz and his continuing influence," Music Perception, Vol. 1, No. 2, 1984, pp. 253-275 (see especially pg. 271)
Warren, R. M., "Subjective Loudness and Its Physical Correlate," Acustica, 37, 1977, pp. 334-346.
Watson, Charles S., Henry W. Wroton, William J. Kelly and Carole A. Benbassat. "Factors in the dicrimination of tonal patterns. I. Component frequnecy, temporal position, and silent intervals," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 57, No. 5, 1975, pp. 1175-1185
Watson, C., Kelly, W. and Wroton, H., "Factors in the discrimination of tonal pattern II. Selective attention and learning under various levels of stimulus uncertainty," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 60, 1976, pp. 1176-1186
Wedin, L. and Goude, G. "Dimnensional analysis of the perception of isntrumental timbres," Scandanavian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 13, 1972, pp. 228-240
Werner, Heinz. "Musical `micro-scales' and `micro-melodies,'" Journal of Psychology, Vol. 10, 1940, pp. 149-156
Wessel, D. & Ehresman, M., "Perception of Timbral Analogies," Rapports IRCAM, 1978
Wessel, D. L, "Psychoacoustics and music: A report from Michigan State University," Bulletin of the Computer Arts Society, Vol. 30, 1973
Wessel, D., Low Dimensional Control of Musical Timbre, Rapports IRCAM, 1978
Wever, E. G., C. W. Bray and M. Lawrence, "The Origin of Combination Tones," J. Exptl. Psychol., Vol. 27, pp. 217--226, 1940
Wever, E. G., C. W. Bray and M. lawrence, " A Quantititative Study of Combination Tones," J. Exptl. Psychol., Vol. 27, pp. 469-496, 1940
Wever, E. G., Theory of Hearing, New York, 1949
White, B. W., "Recognition of distorted melodies," American Journal of Psychooogy, Vol. 73, 1960, pp. 100-107
Whitfield, I. C., "Central Nervous Processing in Relation to Spatio-Temporal Discrimination of Auditory Patterns," in Frequency Analysis and Periodicity Detection in Hearing, R. Plomp and G. F. Smoorenberg, Eds., 1970. A. W. Sijthoff, Leiden, The Netherlands
Wiener, Norbert. "Spatial-Temporal Continuity, Quantum Theory and Music," in W. Capek, ed., The Concepts of Space and Time. Boston: Reidel, 1975.
Wier, C., Jesteadt, W. and Green D. M., "Comparison of method of adjustment and forced-choice procedures in frequency discriminations," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 57, 1975, S27(A).
Wightman, F. I., "The Pattern-Transformation Model of Pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 54, 1973, 407-416
Wundt, W. Grunzeuge der physiologiechsen Psychologie. Leipzig, 1893. 2nd ed., 1902.
Young, R. W., "Inharmonicity of Plain Wire Strings," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 24, 1955, pp. 267-273
Zonis, E., Classical Persian music: An introduction. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press, 1973
Zwicker, E. and Terhardt, E. (eds.), Facts and Models In Hearing. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. 1974.
Zwicker, E., G. Flottorp and S. S. Stevens: "Critical Band Width In Loudness Summation," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 29, pp. 548-557, 1957
Zwicker, E., "Subdivision of the audible frequency range into critical bands," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 33, 1961, pg. 248
Zwicker, E.. "Negative `afterimage' in hearing," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1965, pp. 2413-2415
Zwicker, E., and Scharf, B. "A model of loudness summation," Psychological Review," Vol. 72, 1965, pp. 3-26
Zwicker, E. and H. Fastl, Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models, Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1990.
Zwislocki, J., "Acoustic Attenuation between the Ears," J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 25, 1953, pp. 752-759
Zwislocki, J., R. P. Hellmann and R. T. Verrillo. "Threshold of audibility for short pulses," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 34, No. 10, 1963, pp. 1648-1652